Results and Discussion
H1 predicts that there will be a strong preference
for multi-syllabic brand names preferably three or more
syllables. Table 2 shows that one-syllable brand names
are very rare, only accounting for 0.6 percent,
and multi-syllable brand names altogether account for
99.4 percent in our sample data. Three- or more- syllable
brand names account for 86.2 percent. As predicted in
the Hypothesis 1, there is a strong prefe- rence for
multi-syllable brand names preferably three or more
syllables.

Regarding the order of preference, four-syllable
(25.5%) brand names are the most preferred followed
by three- (21.8%), five- (16.0%) and two-syllable (13.2%)
brand names.
This result is in contrast to Chinese brand names
that are preferably two-syllables. One possible explan-
ation is that two syllables are typical in Chinese words
and brand names in Sino-Korean used to have two syllables,
while relatively recent brand names are made of pure
Korean and have more syllables.
H2 hypothesized that if there are codas, they are
liquids or nasals (l '¤©', n '¤¤', m '¤±', ng '¤·',
swun- mae-sil '¼ø¸Å½Ç'); there is also a strong tendency
to avoid consonant clusters. Being different from our
expectation, the part of a strong preference for syllables
without coda was not confirmed.
Table 3 shows that the brand names without codas
in our data account for only 18.4 percent. The second
half of the H2, however, turns out to be supported since
the majority of the brand names (55.4%) have codas of
nasals and liquids.

It was also found that there is a strong tendency
to avoid consonant clusters. Consequently it is confirmed
that there is a strong tendency that brand names with
coda of liquids/nasals are strongly preferred.
Nasals and liquids in most cases of brand names with
coda are explained for the phonological reason, i.e.,
for the ease of articulation. Avoidance of consonant
clusters is also due to the same phenomenon. There is
a possibility that these two types of consonants as
coda are used specifically more for drinks than any
other products. The present study has a limitation to
present any conclusive answer for the possibility and
we leave it for future research.
H3 states that there will be few mono-morpheme brand
names, and more compounds and wherein the compounding
structure follows the pattern of modifier-noun (yeol-ryang-ml
'¿·®¹Ð'). As shown in Table 4, in terms of compounding
structures, the majority of the brand names in our data
show a preference for a modifier-noun compound name.

The mono-morpheme brand names account for 33.8 percent
while the multi-morpheme brand names account for 66.2
percent. Therefore, it is confirmed that the majority
of brand names in our data have a compounding structure
following the pattern of modifier-noun.
H3 is directly related to H7 in a recent trend for
phrasal names since a phrase consists of more than one
morpheme. But an increase of multi-morpheme names even
on the word level shows that names should not only sound
good but also be meaningful. In other words, a brand
name in recent years tends to con- tain its function,
substance, and origin so that the product can be differentiated
from other brands of the same category.
The general guiding principle of morphological simplicity
in the naming of brands seems to be con- ntradictory
here. But it also appears to be true that the number
of brand names bearing product chara- cteristics is
increasing. As a result, the effect of this morphological
principle can be lessened.
H4 proposes that the compound brand names will have
a positive connotation(hae-co-mi-in 'ÇØÁ¶¹ÌÀÎ'). Table
5 shows that positive connotations of brand names are
clearly demonstrated by the large proportion of the
brand names in our data.

39.4 percent of the brand names have direct and/or
indirect positive meanings; the remaining 60.6 percent
are neutral in meaning. It is quite interesting that
very similar results were found in Chinese naming of
brands analysis (Chan and Huang 2001). Chan and Huang
(2001) reported 32 percent of positive con- notations
and 68 percent of neutral connotations. No negative
connotations were found in either Korean or Chinese.
H5 predicts that there will be a semantic pattern
that the substance property of the brand is preceded
by the function and origin of the brand (sam-kang-hel-ssi-cin-da-e-chwu
'»ï°Çï¾¾Áø´ëÃß'). As indicated in
Table 6, only 22.0 percent of the brands sh-ow the
proposed semantic pattern. Consequently the predic-
tion proposed in Hypothesis 5 is not verified.

One of the proposed linguistic principles is that
brand names may bear product characteristics. The finding
in this study confirmed the principle. However, the
precedence of function and origin followed by substance
of brand names has not yet been verified.
H6 hypothesized that there will be a recent trend
that foreign-like brand names is preferred (kul-ran-bi-a
'±Û¶õºñ¾Æ¡¯). Table 7 shows that foreign-like brand
names in our data account for 54.2 percent; non-foreign-like
brand names account for 45.8 percent.

To test the trend in time difference predicted in
Hypothesis 6, one-way ANOVA has been used to compare
the means of foreign-like brand names across the following
four categories of years: before 1996, 1996 -1997, 1998-1999,
and 2000-2001. As reported in Table 9, there did not
exist statistically significant differ- ences between
the four different time categories (F=0.366, P>0.05).
Additionally, in Table 8, the order of mean scores is
not descending, either.

Therefore, the recent trend proposed in Hypothesis
6 has not been confirmed.
Even though the tendency is not perfectly proportional,
we can say that there is an overall increase in the
number of foreign-like brand names. The small decrease
of foreign-like names in most recent years is probably
related to the increase of phrasal names in that the
latter has to be normal Korean.

Finally H7 states that there will be a recent trend
that phrasal brand names is preferred (nae- chin- kwu-
nun-swul-chin-kwu '³»Ä£±¸´Â ¼úÄ£±¸'¡¯). Table 10 shows
that phrasal brand names only account for 10.2 percent;
non-phrasal brand names account for 89.8 percent.
To test the trend predicted in Hypothesis 7, one-way
ANOVA has been used to compare means of phrasal brand
names across the following four categories of years:
before 1996, 1996-1997, 1998-1999, and 2000- 2001. As
reported in Table 12, there exist statistically significant
differences between the four different time categories
(F=2.928, P<0.05). Therefore, the recent trend proposed
in Hypothesis 7 has been confirmed.


Additionally, in Table 11, the descending order of
mean scores also confirms that it is a recent tendency
in Korean brand naming that phrasal brand names are
preferred.
|